Murder, muddled thinking and multilevel modelling
Martin Cartwright, Gemma Traviss and Andrew Blance on dealing with data hierarchies.
17 September 2012
On 15 July 2005 Sir Roy Meadow, eminent paediatrician, was struck off by the General Medical Council (GMC) for the 'misleading' evidence he gave as an expert witness in the Sally Clark case in 1999. Mrs Clark was wrongly convicted of killing her two baby sons and jailed for life. Meadow told the jury that the chance of two natural unexplained cot deaths in the same family was 1 in 73 million and would only occur once every 100 years in a population the size of England, Wales and Scotland (Hill, 2005). The implication of Meadow's testimony was that the two deaths were not natural events, thereby supporting the prosecution's contention that the babies died at the hands of their mother.
Meadow successfully challenged the GMC decision in the High Court in 2006, with Mr Justice Collins ruling in Meadow's favour on the basis that his testimony did not constitute 'serious professional misconduct'. However, this controversial ruling fully accepted the criticisms of Meadow's statistical evidence. Yet the errors in Meadow's testimony conspicuously reflect flaws in statistical thinking across much contemporary research in medicine and psychology.
The most fundamental error in Meadow's testimony was that he treated the two deaths as occurring independently and squared the probability of a single event occurring to reach the figure of 1 in 73 million for the co-occurrence of two events (RSS, 2001). This calculation fails to recognise that the events occurred in the same context, in this case the family, and the babies therefore shared many influences that could account for their deaths (e.g. genetics, parenting practices). Such shared influences mean that the probability of a second cot death occurring in a family that has already experienced one is higher than a first cot death in a family that has not (Hill, 2004; Watkins, 2000). The true probability of a second cot death occurring in the same family has been estimated to be between 1 in 60 and 1 in 400 but could be as high as 1 in 4 where the genetic influence is strong (Hill, 2005).
Most commonly encountered statistical methods can be seen as special cases of a statistical family known as the generalised linear model (GLM). Analysis of variance (t-test, F-test, ANOVA, ANCOVA, MANOVA, MANCOVA) and regression techniques (linear, logistic, Poisson) can all be derived from the GLM. The underlying statistical equivalence to the GLM explains why these various techniques make similar assumptions about data.
One key assumption shared by all these methods is that the observations or data are independent. In psychology research, data often take the form of observed behaviour or self-report questionnaires. Researchers typically assume the condition of independence has been met if the responses or behaviour of one participant are not observed to directly influence those of another. While this criterion may seem reasonable, it is too narrow for many sampling frames that are encountered in human research and fails to recognise contextual influences on multiple participants that contravene the assumption of independence.
Example: Health anxiety and medical help seeking
Consider a study where researchers examine the link between health anxiety and medical help-seeking. Figure 1a [see PDF] presents a scatterplot of self-reported health anxiety against the frequency of GP visits over the previous year. The graph reveals a positive relationship between the variables (i.e. greater anxiety is associated with more visits). The line of best fit is described by a linear regression equation shown on the graph.
However, this study was not reporting the association between anxiety and help-seeking for 60 participants but for 15 participants who contributed data on four occasions, one year apart (thus 60 data points in total). Given that measures taken at one time point (behaviours, cognitions, emotions, health, socio-economic status, etc.) usually predict the same measures taken at a later time point, data contributed by the same participant on different occasions (e.g. health anxiety measured at Year 1 and Year 2) are temporally related and therefore cannot be truly independent.
Figure 1b shows the same graph but now links data points contributed by five randomly selected participants on four occasions. The fit lines and regression equations for these five participants demonstrate that there are different relationships between health anxiety and GP visits and that the pattern of data points is more similar within participants than between participants. Thus, by examining observations collected from each participant on different occasions, we begin to see structure within the data that was previously erroneously ignored when the data were assumed to be independent.
Furthermore, participants were recruited from five family groups, with three participants per family (5 families x 3 members = 15 participants). Data contributed by participants belonging to the same family are likely to be non-independent, to the extent that concerns about health and medical help-seeking are affected by social influence. Figure 1c highlights all data points contributed by members of each family. This shows that patterns of data are more similar for participants within families than between families.
Dealing with data hierarchies
We have already identified hierarchical data structures in Figure 1: observations (i.e. measurement occasions) nested within individuals and individuals nested within families. This is a three level hierarchical structure but it could have been truncated to two levels if the research was cross-sectional – individuals - (nested within) families, or extended to four levels if the longitudinal study had also collected data about the GPs consulted, observations - individuals - families - GPs.
Once you learn to recognise hierarchies, you start to see them everywhere. In the education system, for example, we see pupils - classes - schools, in psychotherapy we see clients - therapists and in experimental psychology we commonly see time series hierarchies such as repeated measures (e.g. reaction times) - individuals.
There is no limit to the number of levels a hierarchy can have but in all hierarchies a higher-level unit represents a potential contextual influence on all lower-level units. Pupils in the same class, for example, will be exposed to similar influences since they all share the same teacher(s), classroom(s) and social environment. It is these contextual or class-specific influences that lead pupils in the same class to be, on average, more similar than pupils in different classes. Contextual influences may exert influence at all levels of a hierarchy.
It is not only observations between individuals that can be represented as hierarchies but also observations nested within individuals. Unlike traditional methods, multilevel modelling (MLM) approaches are appropriate for designs that require individuals to be assessed on more than one occasion. Time series designs are particularly common in psychology and range from experimental repeated measures laboratory studies (with repeat assessments taken over a period of a few seconds or minutes) through to longitudinal studies (with repeated measures taken over a period of many months or years). Much experimental psychology employs repeated measures designs in which participants complete multiple trials under different conditions using measures such as reaction times, neural activity ERP, EEG, fMRI), skin conductance, eye movements and other psychophysiological outcomes. In all time series designs the individual is not at the bottom of the hierarchy, but higher up with observations (i.e. repeated assessments) nested within the individual.
Given that hierarchies are so pervasive, and assert non-independence of data, researchers need to carefully consider how to deal with such circumstances. Several approaches are available.
Disaggregated approach
A common strategy, and the most unwise, is to simply ignore the data structure. Take the example of a study of academic attainment, with 20 classes each having 30 pupils. A naive (single-level) analysis would assume 600 independent observations and would fail to take into account the two-level structure of the data. Consequently, the proportion of the overall variance that should be attributed to the higher-level units (classes) is misattributed to the lower-level units (pupils), resulting in deflated estimates of standard errors (SEs) and in turn higher than anticipated Type I error rates (rejecting the null hypothesis when it is true, which in the judicial process could be likened to convicting an innocent man).
Aggregated approach
A somewhat different approach is to aggregate data from lower-level units to higher-level units. Accordingly, classes are attributed variables based on aggregated data that was measured at the pupil level. This reduces the units of analysis from 600 (pupils) to 20 (schools). The aggregated approach acknowledges the hierarchical nature of the data but deals with this by dropping the lower level(s) from the analysis altogether. Although this leads to the loss of all lower-level data, the aggregated method is statistically valid provided the data are balanced and inferences are limited to the level of aggregation (class).
In psychology a common example of the aggregated approach occurs when researchers aggregate reaction times (or other psychophysiological measures) over a series of trials and then analyse the data using ANOVA, with each person contributing a mean or median reaction time rather than a series of data. However, whenever the aggregated approach is employed there is always a temptation to draw inferences about lower-level units leading to ecological fallacy (Robinson, 1950). In addition, using the aggregated approach inflates the anticipated rate of Type II errors (failing to reject the null hypothesis when it is false, or setting a guilty man free).
Multilevel modelling
A more sophisticated approach is an extension of standard regression known as multilevel modelling (MLM). MLM exploits the full data structure; therefore no information on the lower level is lost. Furthermore, as MLM explicitly models the hierarchy, it yields correctly specified estimates of the SEs and both Type I and II error rates. MLM also allows considerations of the degree to which lower levels are dependent on higher levels.
The costs of failing to apply appropriate multilevel analysis to data with hierarchical structure can result in highly spurious results. Twisk (2006) compared disaggregated, aggregated and MLM approaches to evaluate a health intervention delivered to patients nested within GPs. When the analyses were repeated with an unbalanced dataset (which is much more likely for real-world research) the estimates of the treatment effect, associated SEs and p-values were substantially different for disaggregated, aggregated and MLM approaches.
Why is MLM important?
The most powerful argument in favour of the MLM approach is simply that it is the correct analysis for hierarchical data because it avoids the distortions of GLM methods. While the concepts underlying MLM are far from simple, the approach offers a great deal of flexibility and overcomes weaknesses of standard single-level techniques. For example, the assumption of homogeneity (that residuals have a constant variance across all values of each covariate) can be relaxed as MLM can explicitly model this variance, which leads to more accurate estimates of fixed effects and their SEs.
As outlined above, multilevel approaches do not require balanced datasets and are particularly good at handling missing values in time series data where, in some circumstances, they perform better than alternative imputation methods. MLM is especially well suited to psychological research, where many outcomes of interest (e.g. mental health, lifestyle behaviours) are a result of both individual (lower-level) and socio-cultural (higher-level) factors. The partitioning of variance between levels of the hierarchy, the accuracy of estimates of fixed effects and the modelling of random variation affords MLM the potential to open doors to new insights into the effects of context on individual phenomena (e.g. development, cognition, emotion, behaviour).
The commonplace failure of researchers to apply appropriate analyses to their data represents the same muddled thinking as evidenced by Roy Meadow in his expert witness testimony. While the immediate consequences of such statistical illiteracy for most psychology researchers may seem more remote than for expert witnesses, the cumulative costs for the discipline are profound. Psychological research not only influences practice and policy in a multitude of areas, but also directs the allocation of resources. It is therefore incumbent on all psychologists, whether consumers or producers of research evidence, to become familiar with multilevel approaches and ensure that these methods are applied where appropriate in our own research, and recognise where others have failed to do this.
Despite the overwhelming justification for multilevel approaches there remain several barriers to widespread adoption. MLM imposes other criteria that can be onerous and costly (e.g. there must be at least 20, and ideally >50, higher-level units). Sampling frames are often not designed with these criteria in mind; therefore much data collected are not suitable for MLM even though there may be good reason to believe that the data are hierarchically structured. Even when the data are suitable, MLM can be difficult to implement and interpret, especially when analysing random factors. Despite these barriers, MLM is becoming more easily accessible. Commercial (SPSS, STATA, SAS) and free (MLwiN, R) software capable of implementing MLM is now widely available, and there are an increasing number of introductory texts and online resources that describe how to implement MLM (e.g. Field, 2009; Field & Miles, 2010; Field & Wright, 2011; Field et al., in press; Gilthorpe et al., 2007; Kreft & de Leeuw, 1998; Wright, 1998; Wright & London, 2009). For those who prefer more formal learning there are also a growing number of short courses available around the UK (e.g. www.leeds.ac.uk/statistical_thinking and www.bristol.ac.uk/cmm/learning/course.html).
With more opportunities to learn about MLM than ever before the time has come for psychology researchers to embrace this flexible approach for exploiting data structure. We should no longer convict the innocent but, instead, ensure we do our research justice!
I Martin Cartwright is at Health Services Research, City University
[email protected]
I Gemma Traviss is at the Leeds Institute of Health Sciences, University of Leeds
[email protected]
I Andrew Blance is at the Leeds Institute of Genetics, Health and Therapeutics, University of Leeds
[email protected]
References
Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS (3rd edn). London: Sage.
Field, A.P. & Miles, J.M.V. (2010). Discovering statistics using SAS. London: Sage.
Field, A.P. & Wright, D.B. (2011). A primer on using multilevel models in clinical and experimental psychopathology research. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 2(2), 271–293.
Field, A.P., Miles, J.M.V. & Field, Z.C. (in press). Discovering statistics using R. London: Sage.
Gilthorpe, M.S., Maddick, I.H. & Petrie, A. (2007). Introduction to multilevel modelling in dental research. Community Dental Health, 17, 222–226.
Hill, R. (2004). Multiple sudden infant deaths – Coincidence or beyond coincidence? Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 18, 320–326.
Hill, R. (2005). Reflections on the cot death cases. Significance, 2, 13–15.
Kreft, I.G.G. & de Leeuw, J. (1998). Introducing multilevel modeling. London: Sage.
Robinson, W.S. (1950). Ecological correlations and the behavior of individuals. American Sociological Review, 15(3), 351–357.
Royal Statistical Society (2001). Royal Statistical Society concerned by issues raised in Sally Clark case. www.sallyclark.org.uk/RSS.html (accessed 16 July 2011)
Twisk, J.W.R. (2006). Applied multi-level analysis: Practical guides to biostatistics and epidemiology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Watkins, S.J. (2000). Conviction by mathematical error? Doctors and lawyers should get probability theory right. British Medical Journal, 320, 2–3.
Wright, D.B. (1998). Modelling clustered data in autobiographical memory research: The multilevel approach. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12, 339–357.
Wright, D.B. & London, K. (2009). Multilevel modelling: Beyond the basic applications. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 62, 439–456.